Healthy Skepticism International News
January 2002
Background for a research proposal
January/February 2002 Vol 20 No 1/2
By Peter R Mansfield
Healthy Skepticism International News
Introduction
Healthy Skepticism is moving into teaching and research. This edition presents the backgrounder for a proposal to investigate the teaching critical appraisal of promotion to change attitudes towards drug promotion and thus improve prescribing. The following commences with a brief overview of the Australian general practice context but the trends are similar in many other countries. After that is a discussion of some risk factors for suboptimal prescribing and the analogy of cardiovascular disease as an aid to thinking about those risk factors and the types of research that are needed. Finally we report the early results of some teaching of critical appraisal of promotion.
We would appreciate any comments on this backgrounder.
Please send comments to .(JavaScript must be enabled to view this email address)
Home: www.healthyskepticism.org
The Australian general practice context
The quality and economy of prescribing by Australian general practitioners (GPs) is highly variable. Looking at costs as an example, in calendar year 1993 the median prescription cost per consultation for “full time” vocationally registered GPs after omitting the lower and upper 5% of the distribution was $9.26. The interquartile range (the middle 50% of cases) was $6.87-$11.73 and the adjusted range was $2.57 - $25.64.[1]
One of the main causes of this variability is differences in case mix. However responses to case vignettes (eg National Prescribing Service case study 5) show wide variations GPs responses to identical “patients”. [2] It appears that different GPs may diagnose similar patients differently and/or treat the same diagnosis differently. Consider the example of antidepressants. Firstly, GPs vary in the tendency to diagnose various forms of distress that do not meet the criteria for major depression as “depression” rather than use other concepts. Secondly, having diagnosed “depression” GPs vary in the tendency to prescribe an antidepressant alone vs non-drug treatment alone vs antidepressant plus non-drug treatment vs no treatment. Thirdly, having decided to prescribe an antidepressant GPs vary in the tendency to prescribe newer more expensive drugs vs older cheaper alternatives.. [3]
Some of the newer drugs are medically superior and sometimes even more cost effective than the older alternatives but many are not. For example COX2 inhibitors are not more effective and appear to have a higher total rate of severe adverse effects than older non-selective NSAIDs. [4] Other example include newer vs older drugs for asthma, diabetes, hyperlipidemia and hypertension.
In financial year 1992-93 the Pharmaceutical Benefits scheme cost taxpayers $1.52 billion and in 1993-94 it cost taxpayers $1.80 billion. [5];[6] By financial year 2000-01 the cost to taxpayers had risen to $3.82 billion, an increase of 19.9% on the previous financial year. The growth in prescriptions volumes was only 7.2% so the main driver of growth is the continuing trend for doctors to change to prescribing newer more expensive drugs. [7] There is increasing concern that this growth rate is not sustainable so there is now a consensus that change is necessary but there is no consensus on what type of changes would be best. [8-10]
On 8 March 2002 Australian Prime Minister John Howard announced there would be major policy changes regarding growth of the PBS. He said the Government had to ensure “proper constraints and proper disciplines because the cost of prescribing these drugs on the so-called free list can grow exponentially if we’re not careful”. Reportedly “the Finance Department is pressing the Health Department to introduce measures over four years to slash yearly spending on the Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme (PBS) by $1 billion a year by 2006.” [11]
Pharmaceutical promotion is a significant cost driver and is “unambiguously bad for welfare”
A major study by Dutch economists of product level data sets for the promotion (detailing, advertising and direct mail) and sales of prescription pharmaceuticals in the Netherlands, for every month in the 1994-1999 period was published in March 2002. The data covered eleven therapeutic markets; together these markets covered 58% of all markets for prescription pharmaceuticals when measured by total sales and 55% when measured by marketing expenditure. They were able to produce a multiple regression model that predicted sales each month with an R squared of 0.87 (ie accounts for 87% of the variation) based on recent sales and recent expenditure on promotion alone. They reported that “marketing expenditures have a statistically significant, positive effect on sales: a 1% marketing increase results in 0.30% more sales (p =0.00). Of this sales rise, approximately 35% is compensated by sales decreases of competing products, the remaining 65% is due to market growth.” However they did find evidence of the law of diminishing returns. Because their study was observational the possibility that the causation relationship may not be as simple as they suggest should be considered. The economists also concluded that pharmaceutical promotion also leads doctors to be “less sensitive to prices when deciding which pharmaceutical he [or she] should prescribe. This is unambiguously bad for welfare.” [12]
Risk factors for suboptimal prescribing
Observational evidence suggests that two of the many independent risk factors that account for variability in prescribing are:
- 1) Exposure to pharmaceutical promotion. (Behaviours such as allowing more visits by drug company representatives, attending more meetings arranged by drug companies, reading more publications funded by drug companies rather than reading high quality medical journals and independent drug bulletins.)
- 2) Positive attitudes towards pharmaceutical promotion. (Beliefs such as “Drug company representatives are a useful source of information”, “Pharmaceutical advertising rarely includes misleading claims”, “Information from drug companies helps me to improve my prescribing”.)
Eleven relevant studies from other countries that Prof Joel Lexchin and I am aware of have found that higher levels of exposure and more positive attitudes towards pharmaceutical promotion are both associated with lower quality and/or economy of actual prescribing or prescribing surrogates (eg choice of drug in response to a case vignette). [13-23] A 3 page tabular summary of these studies written by Joel is available on request. The best of these studies by Berings et al (1994) studied 128 GPs in Flanders. They found that the number of visits from commercial representatives and the estimated utility of commercial information plus year of graduation accounted for 26% of the variation in the tendency to prescribe benzodiazepines. [22] Responses to National Prescribing Service Case Study Number 5 suggest that these findings also apply to Australian general practitioners except in the former study older doctors prescribed more benzodiazepines but in the NPS data older doctors were less positive about inappropriate new drugs. The NPS data suggests that important risk factors for unjustified beliefs about the appropriateness of new drugs included in order of significance:
- 1) More recent graduation.
- 2) Stronger belief that “My prescribing is improved by information from drug companies.”
- 3) Belief that a lower % of drug advertisements contain potentially misleading claims.
- 4) More frequent attendance at drug company sponsored meetings.[3]
A useful analogy
The current state of the art is analogous to that point in medical history when doctors had observed a relationship between pulse pressure and early heart attacks. The corresponding concepts are described in table 1.
Table 1 Using the analogy of early heart attack as a way to think about suboptimal prescribing.
Concepts related to early heart attacks | Concepts related to suboptimal prescribing |
---|---|
Early heart attacks | Poor quality/High cost prescribing |
Pulse pressure | Individual questions on beliefs about drug promotion |
Cigarette smoking | Exposure to promotion |
Nicotine rush | Easy perception of keeping up with new drugs, gifts, social support etc |
Quitline | www.nofreelunch.org (NB nofreelunch uses an analogy to the Temperance approach to alcohol by inviting doctors to take “The Pledge” regardless of passing their “CAGE” test. ) |
Nicotine Replacement Therapy | Best Practice Advocacy Centre, New Zealand |
Blood pressure | Acceptance (vs scepticism) of drug promotion |
The mechanisms via which high blood pressure can lead to early heart attacks | Logical fallacies and the psychology of influence. |
Sphygmomanometer | Questionnaire measuring beliefs about drug promotion |
Thiazides | Interventions that reduce acceptance of drug promotion by teaching critical appraisal of promotion ie how to recognise logical fallacies. |
Beta-blockers | Interventions that reduce acceptance of drug promotion by teaching that receiving gifts from drug companies is harmful/not ethical. |
Age | Older doctors tend to prescribe older drugs whether justified or not and younger doctors tend to prescribe newer drugs whether justified or not. Younger doctors tend to prescribe less drugs per consultation. |
Healthy diet | Independent drug bulletins |
Physical activity | Evidence based medicine critical appraisal skills. |
The aim of the proposed research program is the equivalent of describing some of the mechanisms of hypertension, inventing a sphygmomanometer, and a thiazide and then showing that it works to reduce the rate of early heart attacks. Positive side effects of the thiazide may include reduced “smoking” and improved “diet and physical activity levels” but I am less optimistic of achieving impact via such changes.
 
References
1. Henry, David and O’Connell, Dianne. Variability in prescribing of pharmaceuticals by General Practitioners and factors related to the variability. Final Report of the General Practitioners’ Prescribing Analysis Project (GPPAP) . 1996.
2. Mansfield, Peter and Lexchin, Joel. Case study 5: New Drugs. National Prescribing Service Sydney . 2000.
3. Mansfield, Peter. Year of graduation, level of “scepticism†and attendance at sponsored meetings are significant predictors of General Practioners’ beliefs about the appropriateness of drugs. Poster presented at the National Medicines Symposium, Melbourne . 2000.
4. COX-2 inhibitors update: Do journal publications tell the full story? Therapeutics Letter (43). 2001.
5. PBS Statistics 1992-1993. Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme. Canberra .
6. PBS Statistics 1993-1994. Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme. Canberra .
7. Summary of PBS Processing June 2001. Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme. Canberra .
8. Mansfield, Peter R. Costs are important but it is medicines that save lives. Australian Financial Review 2 February, 2002:
9. White, Annie. What’s wrong with the PBS. Australian Doctor 1 March, 2002, 23-26..
10. Mansfield, Peter. Pushing expensive new drugs. Consuming Interest 2001; 86, 27-29..
11. Metherell. Mark. Patients face drug rations. Sydney Morning Herald 8 March. 2002.
12. de Laat E, Windmeijer F Douven R. How does pharmaceutical marketing influence doctors’ prescribing behaviour? CPB Netherlands ’ Bureau for Economic Policy Analysis. The Hague March 2002.
13. Mapes R. Aspects of British general practitioners’ prescribing. Medical Care 1977;15:371-81.
14. Blondeel, L., Cannoodt, L., De Meyere, M., and Proesmans, H. Prescription behaviour of 358 Flemish general practitioners. 1987.
15. Becker MH, Stolley PD, Lasagna L, McEvilla JD, Sloane LM. Differential education concerning therapeutics and resultant physician prescribing patterns. Journal of Medical Education 1972;47:118-27.
16. Linn LS,.Davis MS. Physicians’ orientation toward the legitimacy of drug use and heir preferred source of new drug information. Social Science and Medicine 1972;6:199-203.
17. Haayer F. Rational prescribing and sources of information. Social Science and Medicine 1982;16:2017-23.
18. Caudill TS, Johnson MS, Rich EC, McKinney WP. Physicians, pharmaceutical sales representatives, and the cost of prescribing. Archives of Family Medicine 1996;5:201-6.
19. Bower AD,.Burkett GL. Family physicians and generic drugs: a study of recognition, information sources, prescribing attitudes, and practices. Journal of Family Practice 1987;24:612-6.
20. Cormack MA,.Howells E. Factors linked to the prescribing of benzodiazepines by general practice principals and trainees. Family Practice 1992;9:466-71.
21. Ferry ME, Lamy PP, Becker LA. Physicians’ knowledge of prescribing for the elderly: a study of primary care physicians in Pennsylvania. Journal of the American Geriatric Society 1985;33:616-22.
22. Berings D, Blondeel L, Habraken H. The effect of industry-independent drug information on the prescribing of benzodiazepines in general practice. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 1994;46:501-5.
23. Powers, R. L., Halbritter, K. A, Arbogast, J. G., Neely, J. L., and Williams, A. J. Do interactions with pharmaceutical representatives influence antihypertensive medication prescribing practices of family medicine and general internal medicine physicians? Journal of General Internal Medicine 1998;(13(supplement)), 13
 
Page views since 15 March 2010: 5615
Comments
Our members can see and make comments on this page.